Please use this identifier to cite or link to this item: http://hdl.handle.net/1893/36361
Full metadata record
DC FieldValueLanguage
dc.contributor.authorMustile, Magdaen_UK
dc.contributor.authorKourtis, Dimitriosen_UK
dc.contributor.authorEdwards, Martin Gen_UK
dc.contributor.authorDonaldson, David Ien_UK
dc.contributor.authorIetswaart, Magdalenaen_UK
dc.date.accessioned2024-10-17T00:10:29Z-
dc.date.available2024-10-17T00:10:29Z-
dc.date.issued2024en_UK
dc.identifier.urihttp://hdl.handle.net/1893/36361-
dc.description.abstractA large body of evidence shows that motor imagery and action execution behaviors result from overlapping neural substrates, even in the absence of overt movement during motor imagery. To date it is unclear how neural activations in motor imagery and execution compare for naturalistic whole-body movements, such as walking. Neuroimaging studies have not directly compared imagery and execution during dynamic walking movements. Here we recorded brain activation with mobile EEG during walking compared to during imagery of walking, with mental counting as a control condition. We asked 24 healthy participants to either walk six steps on a path, imagine taking six steps, or mentally count from one to six. We found beta and alpha power modulation during motor imagery resembling action execution patterns; a correspondence not found performing the control task of mental counting. Neural overlap occurred early in the execution and imagery walking actions, suggesting activation of shared action representations. Remarkably, a distinctive walking-related beta rebound occurred both during action execution and imagery at the end of the action suggesting that, like actual walking, motor imagery involves resetting or inhibition of motor processes. However, we also found that motor imagery elicits a distinct pattern of more distributed beta activity, especially at the beginning of the task. These results indicate that motor imagery and execution of naturalistic walking involve shared motor-cognitive activations, but that motor imagery requires additional cortical resources.en_UK
dc.language.isoenen_UK
dc.publisherFrontiers Media SAen_UK
dc.relationMustile M, Kourtis D, Edwards MG, Donaldson DI & Ietswaart M (2024) Neural correlates of motor imagery and execution in real-world dynamic behavior: evidence for similarities and differences. <i>Frontiers in Human Neuroscience</i>, 18. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2024.1412307en_UK
dc.rightsCopyright © 2024 Mustile, Kourtis, Edwards, Donaldson and Ietswaart. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.en_UK
dc.rights.urihttp://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/en_UK
dc.subjectmotor imageryen_UK
dc.subjectsimulationen_UK
dc.subjectbrain oscillationsen_UK
dc.subjectcognitive processesen_UK
dc.subjectEEGen_UK
dc.subjectfunctional equivalenceen_UK
dc.titleNeural correlates of motor imagery and execution in real-world dynamic behavior: evidence for similarities and differencesen_UK
dc.typeJournal Articleen_UK
dc.identifier.doi10.3389/fnhum.2024.1412307en_UK
dc.identifier.pmid38974480en_UK
dc.citation.jtitleFrontiers in Human Neuroscienceen_UK
dc.citation.issn1662-5161en_UK
dc.citation.issn1662-5161en_UK
dc.citation.volume18en_UK
dc.citation.publicationstatusPublisheden_UK
dc.citation.peerreviewedRefereeden_UK
dc.type.statusVoR - Version of Recorden_UK
dc.contributor.funderSFC Scottish Funding Councilen_UK
dc.author.emailmagdalena.ietswaart@stir.ac.uken_UK
dc.citation.date21/06/2024en_UK
dc.contributor.affiliationPsychologyen_UK
dc.contributor.affiliationPsychologyen_UK
dc.contributor.affiliationCatholic University of Louvainen_UK
dc.contributor.affiliationUniversity of St Andrewsen_UK
dc.contributor.affiliationPsychologyen_UK
dc.identifier.isiWOS:001262041600001en_UK
dc.identifier.scopusid2-s2.0-85197550119en_UK
dc.identifier.wtid2054123en_UK
dc.contributor.orcid0000-0002-7903-3184en_UK
dc.contributor.orcid0000-0003-2535-6196en_UK
dc.contributor.orcid0000-0003-4576-9393en_UK
dc.date.accepted2024-05-20en_UK
dcterms.dateAccepted2024-05-20en_UK
dc.date.filedepositdate2024-10-15en_UK
dc.relation.funderprojectMobile Cognition Innovationen_UK
dc.relation.funderrefN/Aen_UK
rioxxterms.apcnot chargeden_UK
rioxxterms.typeJournal Article/Reviewen_UK
rioxxterms.versionVoRen_UK
local.rioxx.authorMustile, Magda|0000-0002-7903-3184en_UK
local.rioxx.authorKourtis, Dimitrios|0000-0003-2535-6196en_UK
local.rioxx.authorEdwards, Martin G|en_UK
local.rioxx.authorDonaldson, David I|en_UK
local.rioxx.authorIetswaart, Magdalena|0000-0003-4576-9393en_UK
local.rioxx.projectN/A|Scottish Funding Council|http://dx.doi.org/10.13039/501100000360en_UK
local.rioxx.freetoreaddate2024-10-15en_UK
local.rioxx.licencehttp://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/|2024-10-15|en_UK
local.rioxx.filenamefnhum-18-1412307.pdfen_UK
local.rioxx.filecount1en_UK
local.rioxx.source1662-5161en_UK
Appears in Collections:Psychology Journal Articles

Files in This Item:
File Description SizeFormat 
fnhum-18-1412307.pdfFulltext - Published Version1.46 MBAdobe PDFView/Open


This item is protected by original copyright



A file in this item is licensed under a Creative Commons License Creative Commons

Items in the Repository are protected by copyright, with all rights reserved, unless otherwise indicated.

The metadata of the records in the Repository are available under the CC0 public domain dedication: No Rights Reserved https://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/

If you believe that any material held in STORRE infringes copyright, please contact library@stir.ac.uk providing details and we will remove the Work from public display in STORRE and investigate your claim.